However, even

with proper oversight, this may in the end

However, even

with proper oversight, this may in the end be one of the biggest safety hurdles to overcome. In addition to making transplantation of reprogrammed cells affordable and safe, one of the major hurdles thus far left unsolved is to incorporate all of the sequential steps of neuronal differentiation and synaptic development. In particular, forming new projection neurons in the human brain will be a monumental challenge. Consider the case of a Betz cell, which synapses in the lower spinal cord and which is frequently selleck lost in ALS (Udaka et al., 1986). If we were to imagine that the cell body was the size of a tennis ball, the axon would then extend several miles and would be roughly the diameter of

a garden hose. Besides the tens of thousands of dendritic synapses that would find more need to be formed, the axon would need to find its target, starting as a growth cone a considerable distance way. This would all have to transpire within a milieu lacking the guidance cues that are normally present only during a limited window during development. Apart from these practical issues and the host of other intrinsic issues involved in neuronal regeneration and transplantation (accurate cell delivery, potential immune suppression, etc.), there is the growing appreciation that NSCs, whether in vitro or in vivo, have intrinsic specification that may limit the cell types that can be produced upon differentiation (Gaspard et al., 2008, Hochstim et al., 2008, Merkle et al., 2007 and Rakic et al., 2009). Indeed, transplanted hESC-derived neurons seem to obey the in vitro specification program when transplanted in vivo (Gaspard et al., 2008). Beyond this, there was a flurry of findings recently that a small proportion of transplanted cells acquired the pathology of the host tissue (Brundin et al., 2008, Kordower et al., 2008 and Li et al., 2008). Thus, even if we can successfully coax stem cells to replace neurons in vivo, the

battle may already be lost for some of them. Others have taken advantage of the “bystander” or “chaperone” effect of NSCs in transplantation strategies aimed at preventing or ameliorating neurodegeneration (see Breunig et al., 2007 for review). Basically, it has been found that NSCs secrete neurotrophins, MYO10 growth factors, and other beneficial proteins that promote neuronal health and function. For example, it was found that NSCs ameliorated cognitive functions in a model of Alzheimer’s disease not through neuronal replacement but due to their secretion of BDNF (Blurton-Jones et al., 2009). Other groups are taking these properties of transplanted cells and enhancing them with transgenes such as GDNF. In a rat model of ALS, such cells migrated to the sites of degeneration, differentiated into glia, and were able to preserve motor neurons at early and end stages of disease (Klein et al., 2005 and Suzuki et al., 2007).

NLP-1, a buccalin-related peptide, is expressed in a chemosensory

NLP-1, a buccalin-related peptide, is expressed in a chemosensory neuron and acts upon the NPR-11 receptor

in an interneuron to modulate the dynamics of the odor-evoked response in that same chemosensory neuron, suggesting the existence of a feedback connection between the interneuron and the chemosensory neuron (Chalasani et al., 2010). This feedback connection is mediated by an insulin-related peptide (INS-1) secreted by the interneuron (Chalasani et al., 2010). The NLP-12 peptide is expressed specifically this website in a stretch receptor neuron, and loss-of-function mutants of nlp-12 or its receptor (ckr-2) eliminate pharmacologically induced presynaptic potentiation of ACh release at the neuromuscular junction and result

in decreased locomotion rates ( Hu et al., 2011). In addition, imaging analysis of fluorescently tagged NLP-12 suggests that its secretion is stimulated by the pharmacological agent that induces presynaptic potentiation and that stimulation is prevented by a TRP channel mutation that disrupts mechanosensation in the stretch receptor ( Hu et al., 2011). These results support a model in which NLP-12 mediates a feedback loop that couples motor-induced activation of a stretch receptor to the strength of the neuromuscular junction, although future work is required to identify the cellular locus and molecular mechanisms by which CKR-2 receptor activation closes the loop. Neuropeptides also modulate worm reproductive behaviors, including egg laying and copulation. Neuropeptides encoded by the Screening Library flp-1 gene (the first worm neuropeptide gene whose mutation was shown to induce behavioral defects; Nelson et al., 1998) promote transition from the behavioral state of egg retention to active egg laying, as flp-1 loss-of-function mutant worms spend longer in the egg-retaining state than wild-type

worms ( Waggoner et al., 2000). FLP-1 peptide regulation of egg-laying is bidirectional, as flp-1 mutant worms also fail to suppress egg-laying in food-poor environments ( Waggoner et al., 2000). Egg-laying behavior is also modulated by the EGL-6 neuropeptide receptor whose ligands are related FaRPs encoded by the flp-10 and flp-17 genes ( Ringstad and Horvitz, 2008). Terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase These peptides are expressed in sensory neurons that inhibit egg-laying, as when they are ablated, egg-laying is increased, whereas egl-6 is expressed in motor neurons that innervate egg-laying muscles ( Ringstad and Horvitz, 2008). This leads to a simple model in which sensory stimuli relevant to the suitability of the environment for egg-laying control FLP-10/FLP-17 secretion, which directly modulates the activity of the egg-laying motor neurons to promote egg-laying in suitable environments and suppress it when unsuitable.

, 2008 and Winkler et al , 2002) to modify DNA chromatin structur

, 2008 and Winkler et al., 2002) to modify DNA chromatin structure (Walia et al., 1998). The ELP3 ortholog in plants is largely nuclear, however, in yeast and several other species, the protein also localizes to the cytoplasm where it is thought to take part in tRNA modification and acetylation of tubulin; however, the mechanistic details are elusive (Creppe et al., 2009, Solinger et al., 2010 and Versées et al., 2010). Interestingly, ELP3 polymorphisms have been associated with decreased risk

for amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (Simpson et al., 2009), and mutations in ELP1 cause familial dysautonomia (Cheishvili et al., 2011 and Slaugenhaupt and Gusella, 2002). To understand ELP3 function, we have investigated the neuronal

role for Gemcitabine ELP3 in vitro and in vivo. We show that presynaptic ELP3 loss of function results in altered morphology and function BMN 673 research buy of T bars at fruit fly neuromuscular junctions (NMJs), and this occurs in the absence of defects in tubulin acetylation. We find that T bars in elp3 mutants change their structure in favor of forming more elaborate cytoplasmic extensions, that more synaptic vesicles are tethered to these T bars, and that neurotransmitter release becomes more efficient, including a larger readily releasable vesicle pool (RRP). Our data indicate that ELP3 is necessary and sufficient for BRP acetylation in vitro and in vivo, and we propose a model where, similar to acetylation of histones, acetylation of BRP regulates the cytoplasmic extensions of T bars, thereby controlling the capture of synaptic vesicles at active zones and neurotransmitter

release efficiency. We previously isolated two EMS alleles of elp3 (elp31 and elp32) that harbor missense mutations in the acetyltransferase domain ( Simpson et al., 2009) and now created independent null alleles by mobilizing PSUP or-Pelp3KG02386, a P element inserted in through the 5′UTR of elp3. We isolated three different deletions of the elp3 locus (elp3Δ3, elp3Δ4, elp3Δ5) as well as a precise excision (elp3rev) that serves as a genetic control ( Figure 1A). These deletions fail to complement one another, as well as elp31 and elp32, but not lethal alleles of morgue, located 5′ of elp3. Similar to elp3 null mutants (elp3Δ3/elp3Δ4), heteroallelic combinations of the EMS alleles and the P element excision alleles die as early pupae, suggesting that all elp3 alleles we isolated are severe hypomorphic or null alleles ( Walker et al., 2011). To determine if the lethality and phenotypes of the elp3 alleles are solely due to loss of ELP3 function, we created transgenic flies that harbor genomic elp3 rescue constructs ( Figure 1B) ( Venken et al., 2006). The constructs allow expression of a C- or N-terminally GFP-tagged ELP3 under native control ( Venken et al., 2008).

The predominance

The predominance see more of cells concerned with slow movement time scales is in line with an earlier recording study, which also showed that cells did not covary 1:1 with the whisking rhythm and that cells would globally turn off and on with whisking (Carvell et al., 1996). Hill et al. (2011) also show that motor cortical neurons accurately predict whisker movements. Most interestingly,

this covariation of motor cortical activity and whisker movements persist after removal of sensory feedback, implying that it reflects efferent control rather than afferent modulation. This finding differs from data in somatosensory cortex, where the removal of sensory feedback disrupts the comodulation of activity and whisking (Fee et al., 1997). This result is of great significance, because it presents one of the clearest dissociations of vibrissae motor and somatosensory cortical activity in sensorimotor integration discovered so far. The modulation of neural activity associated Metformin price with whisking is fairly weak. Overall there is only a temporal redistribution of neural activity during whisking and no net firing rate increase during whisking! Does such weak modulation argue against a motor role of these neurons? Almost certainly not. In most mammalian motor cortices the activity during spontaneous behaviors is rather modest. The situation changes

when tasks become complicated or when animals are trained on certain movements. One might guess that for most of the day motor cortex is not in the driver’s seat, and instead acts like a mastermind of complicated, unusual, or very significant movements. As for the lesions to the motor cortical forelimb representation performed by Fritsch and Hitzig, damage to vibrissa motor cortex does not fully abolish whisker movements. The persistence of whisking after cortical ablation suggested early on the existence of a brain stem pattern generator for whisking. Lesions to vibrissa motor cortex do affect the amplitude distribution of whisker movements, a result much in line with the current results from Hill et al. (2011). The characteristics

of stimulation-evoked only movements in vibrissa motor cortex strongly depend on methodology of stimulation and the identity of the stimulated neurons (Brecht et al., 2006). Stimulation of pyramidal neurons and interneurons evokes movements of opposite directions. While movements evoked by brief trains of extracellular stimulation pulses are brief and restricted to few whiskers, movement fields observed with single-cell stimulation are large and single-cell-evoked movements persist for seconds (Brecht et al., 2004b). Single-cell stimulation effects are in line with the conclusion of Hill et al. (2011) that vibrissa motor cortex controls movements on long timescales. Vibrissa motor cortex distributes output to a wide variety of subcortical targets. Inputs to vibrissa motor cortex arrive from a wide variety of brain regions in an intricate extremely orderly laminar pattern.

These properties, which we will address below, include (1) cross-

These properties, which we will address below, include (1) cross-orientation

suppression, (2) contrast invariance of orientation tuning width, (3) contrast-dependent changes in response timing and in temporal frequency preference, and (4) the mismatch between measured orientation tuning and the tuning predicted by a simple cell’s receptive field organization. Uncovering the origin of these properties has proven to be one of the keys to this website understanding the nature of the cortical computation. One comprehensive solution to the origin of simple cell nonlinearities was suggested by psychophysics: in the tilt aftereffect illusion, the perceived orientation of a vertical stimulus is shifted away from vertical after prolonged viewing of a slightly oblique stimulus. This result was interpreted to mean that intracortical inhibition, specifically inhibition between cortical neurons of different preferred orientations, sharpened orientation tuning

or even created it de novo (Blakemore and Tobin, 1972). This proposal was strengthened by pharmacological experiments: cortical application of GABAA antagonists cause a broadening of orientation tuning (Sillito, 1975). Cross-orientation inhibition, a form of lateral inhibition (Hartline, 1949), but in the orientation domain rather than the spatial domain, is considered a natural extension of similar mechanisms either observed or proposed to operate throughout the brain. Because of the columnar organization of orientation preference in the cortex, the orientation domain translates into the spatial domain on the cortical surface. selleck compound Cross-orientation inhibition can then emerge from simple, spatially defined rules of cortical connectivity. Cross-orientation inhibition has been proposed to operate in several distinct modes, depending on the orientation dependence and amplitude of inhibitory interconnections.

In attractor models, mafosfamide feedback inhibition forms a set of multistable attractors (Ben-Yishai et al., 1995 and Somers et al., 1995), in which the width of orientation tuning of cortical cells is determined by the lateral extent of cortico-cortical connections. In recurrent models, recurrent excitatory connections amplify feedforward inputs in a way that is sculpted by lateral inhibitory connections (Douglas et al., 1995). Here again, the width of tuning and other aspects of cortical responses are set by intracortical rather than thalamocortical interconnections. In balanced models, strong recurrent excitation and inhibition are thought to balance one another tightly (van Vreeswijk and Sompolinsky, 1998). In addition to explaining many aspects of simple cell behavior, this balance can explain the large variability of cortical spiking responses (Shadlen and Newsome, 1998). In push-pull models, cross-orientation inhibition arises from feedforward inhibition from simple cell-like inhibitory interneurons (Troyer et al., 1998 and Troyer et al.

Gli2A is the primary activator of Shh target genes, Gli3R the mai

Gli2A is the primary activator of Shh target genes, Gli3R the main repressor (Fuccillo et al., 2006). Disruptions to this regulatory system result in tissue-specific defects: in the ventral neural tube, reduced GliA function results in misspecified ventral cell types, whereas in the limb, reduced Gli3R causes polydactyly (Franz,

1994, Hui ZD1839 mw and Joyner, 1993, Johnson, 1967 and Schimmang et al., 1992). Findings from the mutant screen indicated that Shh regulation of Gli protein function depends on the ability of Shh signaling components to associate with and travel through the primary cilium. Mutations in Ift172, Ift88, Ift52, Kif3a, and Dync2h1 cause losses of ventral neuron cell types, consistent with deficient GliA, and polydactyly in the limb, consistent with reduced Gli3R ( Huangfu and Anderson, 2005, Huangfu et al., 2003, Liu et al., 2005 and May et al., 2005). Further evidence confirms that both Gli activator and repressor functions depend on primary cilia ( Cheung et al., 2009, Endoh-Yamagami et al., 2009 and Liem et al., 2009). A fundamental question regarding Shh signaling is the cellular location at which full-length Gli proteins (Gli-FL) are modified to their repressor or activator forms. In Drosophila,

which does not use the primary cilium for Hh signaling, a complex of Cos2, learn more Fused, and Sufu, in the absence of Hh ligand, recruits protein kinase A (PKA), glycogen synthase kinase 3 (GSK3), and casein because kinase 1 (CK1). These kinases phosphorylate full-length cubitus interruptus (Ci), the Drosophila homolog of the

Gli proteins, and Ci-FL is cleaved to generate CiR ( Zhang et al., 2005). The current model of conversion of Gli3-FL to Gli3R, in the absence of Shh, is strikingly similar in the mouse, except that the complex of Kif7, Sufu, and protein kinases forms at the base of the primary cilium ( Goetz and Anderson, 2010). Meanwhile, Ptch1, near the base of the ciliary membrane, prevents entry of functionally significant levels of Smo. In the presence of Shh, Ptch1 binds Shh and moves away from the ciliary membrane, allowing Smo to accumulate in the cilium ( Chen et al., 2009, Corbit et al., 2005, Endoh-Yamagami et al., 2009, Kim et al., 2009, Rohatgi et al., 2007 and Wang et al., 2009a). Smo activation, in turn, causes Kif7, Sufu, and Gli proteins to travel to the tip of the cilium, with Kif7, in particular, required for efficient Gli2 and Gli3 accumulation ( Cheung et al., 2009, Endoh-Yamagami et al., 2009 and Liem et al., 2009). Gli-FL is thus moved away from the kinase complex that promotes conversion to GliR and may be transformed to GliA at the ciliary tip ( Goetz and Anderson, 2010). In a different model, Gli-FL translocates from the cilium to be converted to GliA only in the nucleus ( Humke et al., 2010).

1 and mCD8-GFP in a subset of E564 neurons

1 and mCD8-GFP in a subset of E564 neurons learn more (

Gordon and Scott, 2009). Adult flies were then assayed for constitutive extension, and the frequency distributions of cell-types in extenders and nonextenders were compared ( Figure 2C). Cell-type 1 was highly enriched in extenders and rarely labeled in nonextenders, whereas the other cells were present at similar frequencies in extenders and nonextenders. Additionally, in five animals that displayed constitutive proboscis extension, cell-type 1 was exclusively labeled, demonstrating that silencing of these neurons produces the aberrant behavior. The neurons that inhibit proboscis extension (which we name PERin) have cell bodies and processes in the first leg neuromeres of the VNC and projections to the SOG, the brain region that contains gustatory sensory axons and proboscis motor neuron dendrites (Figures 2D–2G). Labeling with the presynaptic synaptotagmin-GFP marker (Zhang et al., 2002) and the postsynaptic DenMark marker (Nicolaï et al., 2010) indicated that the dendrites of PERin neurons are restricted to the first leg neuromeres, whereas axons are

found in both the SOG and the first leg neuromeres (Figures 2H and 2I). The anatomy of these neurons suggests that they convey information from the leg neuromeres to a region of the fly brain involved in gustatory processing and proboscis extension. Anatomical studies examining the proximity of PERin fibers to gustatory sensory dendrites or proboscis motor axons revealed that PERin neurons do not come into check details close contact with known neurons that regulate proboscis extension (Figure S2; Movies S1 and S2). There are several different contexts in which PERin neurons might modulate feeding initiation. PERin activity might reflect the satiety the state of the animal, such that high activity inhibits feeding initiation when the animal is fed and low activity promotes feeding when the animal is food-deprived. Alternatively, PERin

neurons might directly process gustatory sensory cues, increasing activity in response to bitter compounds to suppress proboscis extension or decreasing activity upon sucrose stimulation to promote extension. A third possibility is that they regulate proboscis extension in response to other cues, such as mechanosensory or somatosensory cues, to inhibit proboscis extension while the animal is engaged in other behaviors. To test whether PERin neurons influence extension probability based on satiety state, we performed cell-attached electrophysiological recordings to monitor the basal firing rate of PERin neurons under fed and food-deprived conditions (Marella et al., 2012). In both conditions, PERin neurons exhibited constant basal activity of ∼14 Hz, indicating that tonic activity in these neurons is not altered by satiety state (Figures 3A and 3B).

05), but not when odors were present at a much lower concentratio

05), but not when odors were present at a much lower concentration of 2 ppm (Figure 8H). This result is consistent with the idea that the cortical suppression of M/T cell responses depends on sufficient levels of bulbar sensory input. Taken together, these data indicate that cortical feedback regulates sensory information processing in the OB primarily by acting as a gating mechanism that enhances odor-evoked M/T

cell inhibition. Here, we use an optogenetic approach to show that cortical feedback find more projections target diverse populations of interconnected OB interneurons. We show that activation of cortical fibers drives disynaptic inhibition of mitral cells via fast, AMPAR-mediated excitation of GCs. However, activation of cortical fibers also elicits disynaptic feedforward inhibition of GCs and the effects of cortical activity on AP firing in GCs varied from excitation to inhibition. Cortically-evoked inhibition

of GCs results from dSACs that receive a higher convergence of inputs from cortical projections than GCs. Despite the potential for opposing actions on interneuron circuits, in vivo recordings reveal that the major effect of activating cortical feedback projections on M/T cells is to accentuate odor-evoked inhibition and reduce AP firing during Enzalutamide the processing of sensory input. We find that cortical feedback projections elicit mitral cell disynaptic inhibition that differs from classical dendrodendritic inhibition triggered by mitral cell activity. First, while mitral cell recurrent and lateral dendrodendritic inhibition is due to a long-lasting (many hundreds of ms) barrage of asynchronous IPSCs (Isaacson and Strowbridge, 1998; Schoppa et al., 1998; Urban and Sakmann, 2002) activation of cortical fibers evokes short-latency inhibition with a briefer time course (<100 ms).

Second, recurrent and lateral dendrodendritic inhibition typically requires the activation of GC NMDARs (Chen et al., 2000; Isaacson and Strowbridge, 1998; Schoppa et al., 1998), while cortically-evoked IPSCs are insensitive Casein kinase 1 to NMDAR antagonists and require AMPAR activation. Our results suggest that GCs are the likely source of cortically-evoked mitral cell inhibition. Cortical projections evoke short latency APs in GCs and fast (<2 ms) EPSCs mediated by Ca2+-impermeable AMPARs. Although NMDARs are also present at GC cortical synapses, AMPAR-mediated transmission is sufficient to drive AP-dependent fast mitral cell inhibition. We also show that when mitral cells are suprathreshold, fast cortically-driven IPSPs can both transiently suppress mitral cell APs and elicit rebound firing. Previous studies found that while small, brief IPSPs promote rebound spiking in mitral cells, larger hyperpolarizations due to summating IPSPs have a purely inhibitory action (Balu and Strowbridge, 2007; Desmaisons et al., 1999).

During each rotation the plane of polarization was rotated by 360

During each rotation the plane of polarization was rotated by 360° (0° defined as the E-vector parallel to the longitudinal body axis of the animal). The LEDs for unpolarized stimulation (ultraviolet [365 nm], green [520 nm],

blue [460 nm]) were attached to the rotation stage via radial arms extending from the zenith, so that each LED pointed toward the animal (angular size: 3°). With every rotation, each LED passed through all possible azimuth directions at constant elevation. Photon flux rate was equal for all unpolarized stimuli. Over the course of experiments, rotation velocity was either set to 30°/s or 60°/s, and both clockwise as well as counterclockwise rotations were applied in direct sequence. For blocking light to the dorsal region of the compound eye, a small Hydroxychloroquine concentration piece of black tape was positioned directly in front of the eye. Identical stimuli were applied before, during, and after

the shielding. For eliminating polarization during control experiments with zenithal unpolarized light, a diffuser was inserted into the light path. Residual polarization during 360° rotations of the polarizer/diffuser was found to be below 5%. Intensity of polarized light was adjusted to match the unpolarized light intensity resulting from insertion of the diffuser. Neuronal responses to rotations of the polarizer as well as to azimuthal rotations of unpolarized light spots were analyzed with custom designed scripts in Spike2 software. Each spike occurring during a rotation was assigned its corresponding angle (either E-Vector or azimuth). These angles were tested for significant CB-839 mouse difference from randomness using the Rayleigh test for axial (E-vector angles) or circular data (azimuth angles). If activity during rotations was significantly different from randomness, the resulting mean angle was defined as the preferred E-vector secondly or azimuth angle of the examined neuron. For circular plots,

spiking activity during rotations was calculated for 10° bins, averaged over all rotations within each neuron, and plotted against E-vector orientation or azimuth angle, respectively. The response amplitude (R) was calculated as described in Heinze et al. (2009). In brief, R is a measure for the summed absolute deviation from mean activity during stimulus application. Thus, the higher the value of R, the stronger is the response to the stimulus. R values for periods without stimulation were obtained to calculate background variability. Statistical comparison between shielded and unshielded stimulus conditions were performed by analyzing R values for each of the conditions. After R values were normalized to the unshielded response value, a paired t test was then used to compare the shielded response to background variability while a one-sample t test against a hypothetical mean of 1 was performed to compare the shielded response against the normalized unshielded response.

We first established that there is a cognitive control

im

We first established that there is a cognitive control

impairment in NVHL rats because this feature closely EX 527 order resembles the core cognitive deficit that can be measured in schizophrenia (Barch et al., 2009; Wobrock et al., 2009). We operationally define cognitive control as the ability to use relevant information and ignore irrelevant information. We measured this ability in adult (P60) NVHL and control rats using the active place avoidance task (Figure 1A). The task requires a rat on a slowly rotating disk-shaped arena to avoid entering a stationary shock zone. In the two-frame variant of the task, the rat must dissociate locations of shock in the spatial frames of the stationary room and rotating arena by using only the relevant room cues and ignoring the irrelevant arena cues to locate the shock zone (Cimadevilla et al., 2001; Kelemen and Fenton, 2010; Wesierska et al., 2005). Adult NVHL rats tested on the two-frame task were impaired compared to sham control rats as assessed both by the learning curve (Figure 1B, left) and the total entrances across all training trials shown as a performance summary (Figure 1B, p = 0.004). Dasatinib mouse Control rats quickly reduced entering the shock zone, whereas NVHL rats required prolonged training to reach the same level of avoidance. Retention of the avoidance after

a 24 hr delay was tested by comparing performance on trial 16 to trial 17. Retention was not impaired in NVHL rats (t8 = 1.83; p = 0.10), suggesting that long-term memory approached normal in the NVHL rats, once they had reached the performance asymptote. We then investigated whether the NVHL improvement in place avoidance to the

level of controls was a sign of remediation that can be transferred to another task. The shock zone location was changed 180°, creating a conflict task variant that normal rats solve by inhibiting avoidance of the original shock location and learning the reversed location of shock. Control rats quickly avoided the reversed shock zone, whereas avoidance in the NVHL rats was severely impaired (Figure 1B, p = 0.002). Although the place avoidance deficit appeared to attenuate with training in constant conditions, the impairment reappeared with changes in which information should be used and ignored. We verified that the two-frame active place nearly avoidance deficit is a cognitive control impairment rather than an impairment of motivation, spatial perception, memory, or navigation, which are essential components of the avoidance behavior (Figure 1C). We used a one-frame control task in which the arena continues to rotate, just as in the two-frame task, but is covered with shallow water to remove the olfactory cues that were present but irrelevant for avoiding shock in the two-frame task (Wesierska et al., 2005). This essentially allows the rat to use the relevant room cues to locate the shock zone without interference from the hidden arena cues.